Animal Cognition

Papers
(The median citation count of Animal Cognition is 4. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2021-09-01 to 2025-09-01.)
ArticleCitations
“Cognition in marine mammals: the strength of flexibility in adapting to marine life”50
Correction to: If horses had toes: demonstrating mirror self recognition at group level in Equus caballus48
Disentangling help-seeking and giving up: differential human-directed gazing by dogs in a modified unsolvable task paradigm25
Does owner handedness influence paw preference in dogs?23
Assessment of predation risk through conspecific cues by anuran larvae21
Thinking about order: a review of common processing of magnitude and learned orders in animals20
House sparrows use learned information selectively based on whether reward is hidden or visible20
Learning to anticipate mate presence shapes individual sex roles in the hermaphroditic pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis20
Nest sanitation as an effective defence against brood parasitism20
Spaced training enhances equine learning performance18
Seasonal changes in problem-solving in wild African striped mice18
Pet dogs (Canis familiaris) re-engage humans after joint activity17
Life is in motion (through a chick’s eye)17
Sex differences in social buffering and social contagion of alarm responses in zebrafish16
Mutual mother–pup vocal recognition in the highly colonial Cape fur seal: evidence of discrimination of calls with a high acoustic similarity16
Injury shortens life expectancy in ants and affects some risk-related decisions of workers16
Cognitive flexibility in a Tanganyikan bower-building cichlid, Aulonocranus dewindti16
Spot the odd one out: do snake pictures capture macaques’ attention more than other predators?15
Grouping rule in tadpole: is quantity more or size assortment more important?14
Horses discriminate between human facial and vocal expressions of sadness and joy14
Lessons learned in animal acoustic cognition through comparisons with humans13
Learning and organization of within-session sequences by pigeons (Columba livia)13
A new protocol for investigating visual two-choice discrimination learning in lizards12
Both age and experience are important for successful problem solving in juvenile fawn-footed mosaic-tailed rats Melomys cervinipes12
A comparative study of mirror self-recognition in three corvid species12
The PROUST hypothesis: the embodiment of olfactory cognition12
Cognitive control of song production by humpback whales12
What drives horse success at following human-given cues? An investigation of handler familiarity and living conditions12
On being a Hydra with, and without, a nervous system: what do neurons add?12
Decision making under risk: framing effects in pigeon risk preferences12
A promising novel judgement bias test to evaluate affective states in dogs (Canis familiaris)12
Incrementing non-matching- but not matching-to-sample is rapidly learned in an automated version of the odor span task11
Penguins perceive variations of source- and filter-related vocal parameters of species-specific vocalisations11
Smart sharks: a review of chondrichthyan cognition11
Impact of environmental complexity and stocking density on affective states of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss)11
Left or right, that is the question: use of egocentric frame of reference and the right-eye advantage for understanding gestural signs in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)11
Context specificity of latent inhibition in the snail Cornu aspersum11
Impact of exploration behavior, aptitude for pellet consumption, and the predation practice on the performance in consecutive live prey foraging tests in a piscivorous species10
Cue polarization and representation in mouse home base behaviors10
Behavioral laterality is correlated with problem-solving performance in a songbird10
Orientation by environmental geometry and feature cues in the green and black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus)10
Overlooked evidence for semantic compositionality and signal reduction in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)10
Searching high and low: domestic dogs’ understanding of solidity10
Testing use of the first multi-partner cognitive enrichment devices by a group of male bottlenose dolphins9
Volumetric and connectivity assessment of the caudate nucleus in California sea lions and coyotes9
After 150 years of watching: is there a need for synthetic ethology?9
Tolerant macaque species are less impulsive and reactive9
Individual recognition and long-term memory of inanimate interactive agents and humans in dogs9
Host parent responses to heterospecific parasite nestling alarm calls are independent of past and current experience with experimental brood parasitism9
Context-dependent use of olfactory cues by foragers of Vespula germanica social wasps9
Are you better than me? Social comparisons in carrion crows (Corvus corone)9
Basal cognition: shifting the center of gravity (again)9
Can rats and ants exchange information between the horizontal and vertical domains?9
Evidence of successive negative contrast in terrestrial toads (Rhinella arenarum): central or peripheral effect?9
Emulative learning of a two-step task in free-ranging domestic pigs9
Plasticity for the kin and conspecific preferences in the frog tadpoles (Rana ornativentris)9
Visual perception of emotion cues in dogs: a critical review of methodologies8
Nest box entrance hole size can influence nest site selection and nest defence behaviour in Japanese tits8
The deterrent effects of individual monoterpene odours on the dietary decisions of African elephants8
An exploratory analysis of head-tilting in dogs8
African jewel fish (Hemichromis bimaculatus) distinguish individual faces based on their unique iridophore patterns8
Behavioural synchronization and social referencing of dogs and humans: walking in dyad vs in group8
First record of geophagy in Jungle Babbler (Turdoides striata) prior to egg-laying: an instinct for calcium supplement8
Novel object recognition in Octopus maya8
Performance on inhibitory tasks does not relate to handedness in several small groups of Callitrichids8
Varieties of visual navigation in insects8
Mirror self-recognition in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): a review and evaluation of mark test replications and variants8
Wild skuas can use acoustic cues to locate hidden food8
Intra- and interspecific variation in self-control capacities of parrots in a delay of gratification task8
Social experience drives the development of holistic face processing in paper wasps7
Through the eyes of a hunter: assessing perception and exclusion performance in ground-hornbills7
Cognition of the manatee: past research and future developments7
Slowly walking down to the more food: relative quantity discrimination in African spurred tortoises (Centrochelys sulcata)7
Puppies in the problem-solving paradigm: quick males and social females7
Bold and bright: shy and supple? The effect of habitat type on personality–cognition covariance in the Aegean wall lizard (Podarcis erhardii)7
Count-based decision-making in mice: numerosity vs. stimulus control7
Quantity discrimination by kittens of the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus)7
Meowing dogs: can dogs recognize cats in a cross-modal violation of expectancy task (Canis familiaris)?7
Domestication effects on social information transfer in chickens7
Repeated testing does not confound cognitive performance in the Western Australian magpie (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis)7
Juvenile vervet monkeys rely on others when responding to danger7
Gender differences in animal cognition science7
Visual categories and concepts in the avian brain7
Assessing the performance of brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) on the Mechner counting procedure7
Adjustment in the point-following behaviour of free-ranging dogs – roles of social petting and informative-deceptive nature of cues6
Effects of predation risk on the sensory asymmetries and defensive strategies of Bufotes balearicus tadpoles6
Correction: Do chimpanzees see a face on Mars? A search for face pareidolia in chimpanzees6
Interspecific differences in developmental mode determine early cognitive abilities in teleost fish6
Grey wolves (Canis lupus) discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human voices6
Does quantity matter to a stingless bee?6
Horses form cross-modal representations of adults and children6
Object use in communication of semi-wild chimpanzees6
Oscillatory extraction behaviour suggests functional attributes of crows’ hooked-stick tools6
Innovative problem-solving in a small, wild canid6
Two-action task, testing imitative social learning in kea (Nestor notabilis)6
No evidence of spatial representation of age, but “own-age bias” like face processing found in chimpanzees6
When should we ascribe sentience to animals? A commentary on “Hermit crabs, shells and sentience” (Elwood 2022)6
Orcas remember what to copy: a deferred and interference-resistant imitation study6
Active oscillations in microscale navigation6
Context-effect bias in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.): exploring decoy influences in a value-based food choice task6
Breed group differences in the unsolvable problem task: herding dogs prefer their owner, while solitary hunting dogs seek stranger proximity6
Color is necessary for face discrimination in the Northern paper wasp, Polistes fuscatus6
Operant conditioning in antlion larvae and its impairment following exposure to elevated temperatures5
Prior experience modifies acquisition trajectories via response–strategy sampling5
The current state of carnivore cognition5
Vocal signals with different social or non-social contexts in two wild rodent species (Mus caroli and Rattus losea)5
Nest size matters: common cuckoos prefer to parasitize larger nests of Oriental reed warblers5
Perceptual grouping and detection of trial-unique emergent structures by pigeons5
The influence of visual illusion perception on numerosity estimation could be evolutionarily conserved: exploring the numerical Delboeuf illusion in humans (Homo sapiens) and fish (Poecilia reticulata5
Why wait to mark? Possible reasons behind latency from olfactory exploration to overmarking in four African equid species5
Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: a theory5
An investigation on the olfactory capabilities of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)5
No coordination required for resources allocation during colony fission in a social insect? An individual-based model reproduces empirical patterns5
Mirror image stimulation could reverse social-isolation-induced aggressiveness in the high-level subsocial lactating spider5
Mice make temporal inferences about novel locations based on previously learned spatiotemporal contingencies5
Mother–pup recognition mechanisms in Australia sea lion (Neophoca cinerea) using uni- and multi-modal approaches5
Recovery-from-extinction effects in an anuran amphibian: renewal effect, but no reinstatement5
Star finches Neochmia ruficauda have a visual preference for white dot patterns: a possible case of trypophilia5
What is it like to be a choanoflagellate? Sensation, processing and behavior in the closest unicellular relatives of animals5
A small step for rats alters spatial behavior: rats on a bi-level arena explore each level separately5
First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris)5
Geckos differentiate self from other using both skin and faecal chemicals: evidence towards self-recognition?5
Stepwise learning of compound multidimensional visual stimuli by sorting out the dimensions of which they are composed in pigeons5
Context-dependent and seasonal fluctuation in bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) vocalizations4
The use of an intermittent schedule of reinforcement to evaluate detection dogs’ generalization from smokeless-powder4
Azure-winged Magpies would rather avoid losses than strive for benefits based on reciprocal altruism4
Animal cognition, past present and future, a 25th anniversary special issue4
Structural environmental enrichment and the way it is offered influence cognitive judgement bias and anxiety-like behaviours in zebrafish4
A comparison of personality traits of gifted word learner and typical border collies4
Does social motivation mitigate fear caused by a sudden sound in horses?4
On the role of training in delay of gratification paradigms: a reply to Pepperberg 20224
Why an animal needs a brain4
Detecting surface changes in a familiar tune: exploring pitch, tempo and timbre4
Zebrafish excel in number discrimination under an operant conditioning paradigm4
Searching for nothing: placing zero on the temporal continuum4
Come with me: experimental evidence for intentional recruitment in Tonkean macaques4
Navigational roots of spatial and temporal memory structure4
Well-developed spatial reversal learning abilities in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina)4
Use of anvils by the Great Kiskadee (Aves, Tyrannidae): a description based on citizen science data4
“The song remains the same”: not really! Vocal flexibility in the song of the indris4
Use of the sun compass by monocularly occluded homing pigeons in a food localisation task in an outdoor arena4
Heterospecific eavesdropping on disturbance cues of a treefrog4
Fish self-awareness: limits of current knowledge and theoretical expectations4
Spontaneous object recognition in capuchin monkeys: assessing the effects of sex, familiarization phase and retention delay4
Under pressure: the interaction between high-stakes contexts and individual differences in decision-making in humans and non-human species4
Congratulations to Animal Cognition on its 50th birthday! Some thoughts on the last 50 years of animal cognition research4
Stressfulness of the design influences consistency of cognitive measures and their correlation with animal personality traits in wild mice (Mus musculus)4
Intentional gestural communication amongst red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus)4
Llamas use social information from conspecifics and humans to solve a spatial detour task4
Bioelectric networks: the cognitive glue enabling evolutionary scaling from physiology to mind4
Language preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)4
0.08679986000061