Journal of Comparative Psychology

Papers
(The median citation count of Journal of Comparative Psychology is 0. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2021-02-01 to 2025-02-01.)
ArticleCitations
Supplemental Material for Influence of Group Size on Shelter Choice in Blaptica dubia Cockroaches16
Supplemental Material for Cognitive Flexibility and Aging in Coyotes (Canis latrans)15
Supplemental Material for Is Economic Risk Proneness in Young Children (Homo sapiens) Driven by Exploratory Behavior? A Comparison With Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella)11
Supplemental Material for Gaze in Cats (Felis catus) and Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)11
Supplemental Material for A Modified Version of the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task Tests Cognitive Flexibility in Children (Homo sapiens) and Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)11
Acknowledgments10
What enables “distraction” to reduce delay discounting for pigeons (Columba livia).10
Behavioral and hormonal changes following social instability in young rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).9
African striped mice (Rhabdomys) mind the neighbors.9
Cognitive flexibility and aging in coyotes (Canis latrans).9
Individual variation in Black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) foraging behavior in response to a predator model.9
Testing the role of macaque social tolerance in ability to follow human eye gaze.9
Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls.8
Sensitivity to line-of-sight in tolerant versus despotic macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta).8
Same-different conceptualization in dogs (Canis familiaris).8
Assessing the perception of face pareidolia in children (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).8
Stone affordances as potential for action expression in object play in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).7
Tolerance of muzzle contact underpins the acquisition of foraging information in vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus).7
Effects of ethanol ingestion on aversive conditioning in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.).5
Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires.5
Sexual dimorphism in classical conditioning? Sex differences in neophobia, latent inhibition, generalization, and extinction for rats (Rattus norvegicus) in a conditioned taste aversion preparation ir5
Spatial frequency and global–local visual processing in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and humans (Homo sapiens).5
Cross-modal perception of puppies and adult conspecifics in dogs (Canis familiaris).5
Pigeons’ (Columba livia) intertemporal choice in binary-choice and patch-leaving contexts.4
Testing three primate species’ attentional biases toward preferred and unpreferred foods: Seeing red or high valued food?4
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with better task-based delay of gratification skills are rated as less impulsive, more agreeable, and smarter.4
Dogs (Canis familiaris) prioritize independent exploration over looking back.4
How environmental unpredictability and harshness affect chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in risk-choice and temporal discounting tasks.4
Still face in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).4
Cyclical variations in acoustic features within the song sessions of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae).4
Don't let the pigeon chair the search committee: Pigeons (Columba livia) match humans’ (Homo sapiens) suboptimal approach to the secretary problem.4
Beyond the information (not) given: Associative mechanisms versus representations of uncertainty in extinction in laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus).4
Supplemental Material for Testing Three Primate Species’ Attentional Biases Toward Preferred and Unpreferred Foods: Seeing Red or High Valued Food?4
Supplemental Material for Variation in Neophilia in Seven Primate Species4
Learning simple and compound stimuli in a social lizard (Egernia stokesii).3
Adaptable navigation in bull ants (Myrmecia midas).3
Positive intonation increases the perceived value of smaller rewards in a quantity discrimination task with dogs (Canis familiaris).3
Sound order discrimination in two species of birds–Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus.3
Supplemental Material for Assessing Cats’ (Felis catus) Sensitivity to Human Pointing Gestures3
Supplemental Material for Dominance in Human (Homo sapiens) Personality Space and in Hominoid Phylogeny3
The role of head and body cues in visual individual recognition in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus).3
Simplicity and complexity in human and nonhuman communication.3
Supplemental Material for Mother–Infant Relationships and Infant Independence in Wild Geoffroy’s Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi)3
Supplemental Material for Is Inferential Reasoning a Distinctly Human Cognitive Feature? Testing Reasoning in Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)3
Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): A systematic review and meta-analysis.3
Are you as fooled as I am? Visual illusions in human (Homo) and nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) primate species.3
Mother–infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy’s spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi).3
Recurrence quantification analysis in the study of online coordination in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus).3
Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax)3
Supplemental Material for The Dynamics of Chunking in Humans (Homo sapiens) and Guinea Baboons (Papio papio)3
Supplemental Material for Implementation of Automated Cognitive Testing Systems for Socially Housed Rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Squirrel (Saimiri spp.) Monkeys: Age Differences in Learning3
Supplemental Material for Evaluation of Decision-Making Behavior Under Uncertainty in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella) and Humans (Homo sapiens) Using a Modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task2
Iberian Lynx (Lynx pardinus) personality: A rating assessment with ex Situ Conservation Program sample.2
Why do distractions sometimes aid self-control? Pigeons (Columba livia) highlight possible mechanisms underlying the distraction effect.2
Primate origins of corepresentation and cooperative flexibility: A comparative study with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), brown capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana2
Drawing in nonhuman primates: What we know and what remains to be investigated.2
A reflection on noncognitive factors affecting spatial cognitive testing: Examples from nonmodel species.2
Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species?2
A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification.2
How do communicative cues shape the way that dogs (Canis familiaris) encode objects?2
Environmental correlates of exploratory behavior and anxiety in three African striped mouse (Rhabdomys) taxa occurring in different habitats and contexts.2
Humans’ (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency.2
Gaze in cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).2
Conflict interference in an insect.2
How comparative was (is) the Journal of Comparative Psychology? A reptilian perspective.1
Preference or flexibility? Spatial navigation by Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) in a multiple-cue environment.1
I am as fooled as you are, say some primates … but only sometimes.1
Recognition of visual kinship signals in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by humans (Homo sapiens).1
Multiple assessments of personality and problem-solving performance in captive Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) and African savanna elephants (Loxodonta africana).1
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) exhibit gaze bias for snakes upon hearing alarm calls.1
Zing-Yang Kuo and “Giving up instincts in psychology”, 100 years later.1
Supplemental Material for Consistent Second-Order Motor Planning by Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence From a Dowel Task1
Supplemental Material for Are You as Fooled as I am? Visual Illusions in Human (Homo) and Nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) Primate Species1
Looking back to look forward: Avenues into the field of comparative psychology.1
What makes the ephemeral reward task so difficult?1
Supplemental Material for Hearing “Number”?: Relative Quantity Judgments Through the Echolocation by Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)1
How tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and humans (Homo sapiens) handle a jointed tool.1
Comparative psychology’s founding mother, Margaret Floy Washburn.1
Supplemental Material for Inhibitory Control and Cue Relevance Modulate Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) Performance in a Spatial Foraging Task1
Have we met? New insights into the role of head and body cues in the visual recognition of conspecifics in gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus).1
Anthropomorphism as a contributor to the success of human (Homo sapiens) tool use.1
Personality structure in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).1
Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) use a visual barrier for cache protection.1
Acknowledgment1
Great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Pongo abelii) exploit better the information of failure than capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) when selecting tools to solve the same foraging problem.1
Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) internal arousal remains elevated if they cannot themselves help a conspecific.0
Supplemental Material for Do Standard Behavioral Assays Predict Foraging Behavior of Individual Black-Capped Chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in Response to a Predator Model or Calls?0
Comparative cognition: Perspectives, challenges, and prospects.0
Supplemental Material for Physiological Constraints and Cognitive Chunking: Sequence Organization in the Songs of Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata)0
Supplemental Material for Evidence of Motor Intentions in Plants: A Kinematical Study0
Correction to Burghardt (2021).0
Convergence between G and g in three monkey species (Sapajus spp, Ateles geoffroyi, and Macaca fascicularis).0
A century of the Journal of Comparative Psychology.0
Computerized intertemporal choice task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with/without postreward delay.0
Exclusion by donkey’s ears: Donkeys (Equus asinus) use acoustic information to find hidden food in a two-way object-choice task.0
Some phenomena of the cap-pushing response in honey bees (Apis mellifera spp.).0
Sociability of Indian free-ranging dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) varies with human movement in urban areas.0
Forgetting curve in spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi): Effects of delay but not the amount of information.0
Factors in forgetting in spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi.0
Influence of group size on shelter choice in Blaptica dubia cockroaches.0
Food calls enhance visual discrimination learning in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus).0
Children (Homo sapiens), but not rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), perceive the one-is-more illusion.0
Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task.0
Visual discrimination of size and perception of the Delboeuf illusion in the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus): A developmental disjunction?0
Similarities in composition and transformations of songs by humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) over time and space.0
Supplemental Material for Putting the Best Foot Forward: Limb Lateralization in the Goffin’s Cockatoo (Cacatua goffiniana)0
End the search quickly: Pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) share the same bias.0
Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny.0
Supplemental Material for The Effect of Delta-9 Tetrahydrocannabinol and Methamphetamine on Sustained Attention in the Jumping Spider (Trite planiceps)0
Supplemental Material for The Role of Head and Body Cues in Visual Individual Recognition in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus)0
Are chimpanzees futurists? Effects of motion lines and motion blur on the judgments of global motion direction in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).0
The dynamics of chunking in humans (Homo sapiens) and Guinea baboons (Papio papio).0
Supplemental Material for Conflict Interference in an Insect0
Expanding the landscape of opportunity: Professional societies support early-career researchers through community programming and peer coaching.0
Responses to prey chemical cues in wild-caught, adult gopher snakes (Pituophis catenifer).0
(Re)claiming plants in comparative psychology.0
Supplemental Material for Sensitivity to Line-of-Sight in Tolerant Versus Despotic Macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta)0
Guatemalan beaded lizards (Helodermatidae: Heloderma charlesbogerti) navigate and follow a scent trail in maze tasks.0
Delayed gratification: A grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) will wait for more tokens.0
Serial pattern learning: Pigeons (Columba livia) prefer an improving schedule over an initially easier fixed ratio schedule.0
Supplemental Material for Primate Socio-Ecology Shapes the Evolution of Distinctive Facial Repertoires0
Studying animal innovation at the individual level: A ratings-based assessment in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] sp.).0
Hearing “number”? Relative quantity judgments through the echolocation by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).0
No evidence of attentional bias toward threatening conspecific and allospecific faces in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) using a dot-probe task.0
Parental divorce in childhood is related to lower urinary oxytocin concentrations in adulthood.0
Is economic risk proneness in young children (Homo sapiens) driven by exploratory behavior? A comparison with capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).0
Investigation of mirror-self recognition in ravens (Corvus corax).0
Supplemental Material for Primate Origins of Corepresentation and Cooperative Flexibility: A Comparative Study With Common Marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), Brown Capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean0
Extraction of structural regularities by baboons (Papio Papio): Adjacent and nonadjacent repetition patterns differ in learnability.0
Supplemental Material for The Effects of Goal–Landmark Distance on Overshadowing: A Replication in Humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004)0
One hundred years of the Journal of Comparative Psychology.0
Supplemental Material for Still Face in Pet Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Supplemental Material for Vocal Repertoire and Auditory Sensitivity of White-Throated Woodrats (Neotoma albigula)0
Rules and metarules: Adult cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and 5-year-old children (Homo sapiens) can master both.0
Visual and acoustic exploratory behaviors toward novel stimuli in Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus) under human care.0
Is inferential reasoning a distinctly human cognitive feature? Testing reasoning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).0
Physiological constraints and cognitive chunking: Sequence organization in the songs of zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata).0
Target number influences strategy use by rats (Rattus norvegicus) in the traveling salesperson problem.0
First report of reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus) response to human-given cues.0
Vocal repertoire and auditory sensitivity of white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula).0
The journey in comparative psychology matters more than the destination.0
Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) modify caching behavior when observed.0
Initial evidence for eliciting contrafreeloading in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) via the opportunity for playful foraging.0
Supplemental Material for Are Chimpanzees Futurists? Effects of Motion Lines and Motion Blur on the Judgments of Global Motion Direction in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)0
Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.0
100 years of comparative psychology advancing practice, policy, and the public—And what the future requires.0
Supplemental Material for Pitch Affects Human (Homo sapiens) Perception of Emotional Arousal From Diverse Animal Calls0
Putting the best foot forward: Limb lateralization in the Goffin’s cockatoo (Cacatua goffiniana).0
On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can.0
What is it like to hear quantities? Testing dolphins on classic number estimation using a sonar setup.0
Editorial.0
Differences in paradoxical choice between pigeons (Columba livia) and rats (Rattus norvegicus): The problem of cue trackability.0
Facial displays in red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): Repertoire, social context, and potential intentionality.0
Fins, feathers, fingers, and finding an explanation for the puzzle of ephemeral rewards.0
Detouring while foraging up a tree: What bull ants (Myrmecia midas) learn and their reactions to novel sensory cues.0
Supplemental Material for Exclusion by Donkey’s Ears: Donkeys (Equus asinus) Use Acoustic Information to Find Hidden Food in a Two-Way Object-Choice Task0
The coding of object thickness in plants: When roots matter.0
Contrafreeloading in umbrella cockatoos (Cacatua alba): Further evaluation of the play hypothesis.0
The roles of social information, asocial information, and initial bias in nest-building decisions.0
Supplemental Material for Recognition of Visual Kinship Signals in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by Humans (Homo sapiens)0
Supplemental Material for The Roles of Social Information, Asocial Information, and Initial Bias in Nest-Building Decisions0
Evolutionary changes in the predatory attack of carnivorous rodents: A comparative analysis emphasizing grasshopper mice (Onychomys spp.).0
Individual differences guide mate choice in the fighting fish (Betta splendens).0
Evidence of motor intentions in plants: A kinematical study.0
Effect of resource-associated signals on producer responses: Insights from golden fish (Carassius auratus) and an agent-based model.0
Individual recognition of monkey (Macaca fuscata) and human (Homo sapiens) images in primatologists.0
What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication.0
Supplemental Material for Search Asymmetries for Threatening Faces in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)0
Supplemental Material for Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Exhibit Gaze Bias for Snakes Upon Hearing Alarm Calls0
Supplemental Material for Humans’ (Homo sapiens), Capuchin Monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and Rhesus Macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) Size Judgments Shift When Stimuli Change in Frequency0
The impact of training method on odor learning and generalization in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).0
Supplemental Material for A Study of Executive Function in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience Can Affect Delay of Gratification0
Supplemental Material for Impulsivity as a Trait in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris): A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis0
A modified version of the dimensional change card sort task tests cognitive flexibility in children (Homo sapiens) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).0
The “avoid the empty cup” hypothesis does not explain great apes’ (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) responses in two three-cup one-item inference by exclusion tasks.0
A new look at universals and specificities in the songs of humpback whales.0
Search asymmetries for threatening faces in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).0
Supplemental Material for Reach-to-Grasp Kinematic Signatures in Colombian Spider Monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris)0
The effects of goal–landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004).0
Do that again! Memory for self-performed actions in dogs (Canis familiaris).0
Decision-making by bushveld gerbils (Gerbilliscus leucogaster).0
Egg burial in the ringneck dove (Streptopelia risoria): A potential laboratory model system for egg-rejection research?0
Assessing cats’ (Felis catus) sensitivity to human pointing gestures.0
Supplemental Material for Contrafreeloading in Umbrella Cockatoos (Cacatua alba): Further Evaluation of the Play Hypothesis0
Doing reliable research in comparative psychology: Challenges and proposals for improvement.0
Supplemental Material for Delayed Gratification: A Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) Will Wait for More Tokens0
Consistent second-order motor planning by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence from a dowel task.0
Supplemental Material for No Evidence of Attentional Bias Toward Threatening Conspecific and Allospecific Faces in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) Using a Dot-Probe Task0
Implementation of automated cognitive testing systems for socially housed rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and squirrel (Saimiri spp.) monkeys: Age differences in learning.0
Supplemental Material for Effect of Resource-Associated Signals on Producer Responses: Insights From Golden Fish (Carassius auratus) and an Agent-Based Model0
The effect of delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol and methamphetamine on sustained attention in the jumping spider (Trite planiceps).0
Acknowledgment of Ad Hoc Reviewers0
Supplemental Material for Cyclical Variations in Acoustic Features Within the Song Sessions of Humpback Whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)0
Supplemental Material for Sound Order Discrimination in Two Species of Birds–Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus0
Animacy perception in dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans (Homo sapiens): Comparison may be perturbed by inherent differences in looking patterns.0
Reach-to-grasp kinematic signatures in Colombian spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris).0
Incorporating animal agency into research design could improve behavioral and neuroscience research.0
Supplemental Material for Cross-modal Perception of Puppies and Adult Conspecifics in Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
The effects of positive and negative experiences on subsequent behavior and cognitive performance in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella).0
Variation in neophilia in seven primate species.0
Supplemental Material for Detouring While Foraging Up a Tree: What Bull Ants (Myrmecia midas) Learn and Their Reactions to Novel Sensory Cues0
Prior experience mediates the usage of food items as tools in great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo abelii).0
Supplemental Material for Using Isochrony, but Not Meter, to Discriminate Rhythmic Sequences in Rats (Rattus norvegicus)0
Using isochrony, but not meter, to discriminate rhythmic sequences in rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Supplemental Material for Positive Intonation Increases the Perceived Value of Smaller Rewards in a Quantity Discrimination Task With Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Supplemental Material for Spatial Frequency and Global–Local Visual Processing in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and Humans (Homo sapiens)0
Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls?0
Supplemental Material for Guatemalan Beaded Lizards (Helodermatidae: Heloderma charlesbogerti) Navigate and Follow a Scent Trail in Maze Tasks0
Together again but no need to play: Dissociating effects of isolation and separation on social interaction in female rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
Should I stay or should I go? Pigeons’ (Columba livia) performance of a foraging task has implications for optimal foraging theory and serial pattern learning.0
Responses of wild skuas (Catharacta antarctica ssp. lonnbergi) to human cues in cooperative and competitive social contexts.0
0.14332604408264