Journal of Comparative Psychology

Papers
(The median citation count of Journal of Comparative Psychology is 0. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2021-09-01 to 2025-09-01.)
ArticleCitations
Cross-modal perception of puppies and adult conspecifics in dogs (Canis familiaris).19
Testing the waters: Attempts by wild killer whales (Orcinus orca) to provision people (Homo sapiens).14
Pitch affects human (Homo sapiens) perception of emotional arousal from diverse animal calls.14
Supplemental Material for The Dynamics of Chunking in Humans (Homo sapiens) and Guinea Baboons (Papio papio)13
Humans’ (Homo sapiens), capuchin monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and rhesus macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) size judgments shift when stimuli change in frequency.13
Supplemental Material for Assessing Cats’ (Felis catus) Sensitivity to Human Pointing Gestures13
What makes the ephemeral reward task so difficult?12
Personality structure in wild Eastern chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii).10
Exploring personality structure and similarity in coppery titi monkeys (Plecturocebus cupreus).8
Supplemental Material for Reach-to-Grasp Kinematic Signatures in Colombian Spider Monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris)8
Supplemental Material for The Effect of Delta-9 Tetrahydrocannabinol and Methamphetamine on Sustained Attention in the Jumping Spider (Trite planiceps)7
Search asymmetries for threatening faces in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).6
Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris)5
Is inferential reasoning a distinctly human cognitive feature? Testing reasoning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).5
Supplemental Material for Evidence of Motor Intentions in Plants: A Kinematical Study5
Influence of group size on shelter choice in Blaptica dubia cockroaches.5
Assessing cats’ (Felis catus) sensitivity to human pointing gestures.4
Contrafreeloading in umbrella cockatoos (Cacatua alba): Further evaluation of the play hypothesis.4
Eyes or nose: Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) prefer vision over olfaction when searching for food.4
Convergence between G and g in three monkey species (Sapajus spp, Ateles geoffroyi, and Macaca fascicularis).4
Supplemental Material for The Art of Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Diplomacy: Unraveling the Secrets of Successful Negotiations Using Machine Learning4
The effects of goal–landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004).4
Supplemental Material for Using Isochrony, but Not Meter, to Discriminate Rhythmic Sequences in Rats (Rattus norvegicus)4
Detouring while foraging up a tree: What bull ants (Myrmecia midas) learn and their reactions to novel sensory cues.4
Touching faces: The active role of infant capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) in their social development.4
Do standard behavioral assays predict foraging behavior of individual Black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in response to a predator model or calls?3
Cognitive flexibility and aging in coyotes (Canis latrans).3
Effects of ethanol ingestion on aversive conditioning in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.).3
Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) rank lists using multiple cognitive mechanisms simultaneously.3
Is economic risk proneness in young children (Homo sapiens) driven by exploratory behavior? A comparison with capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).3
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with better task-based delay of gratification skills are rated as less impulsive, more agreeable, and smarter.3
Vocal signals produced by the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) in highly motivating contexts.3
Pigeons’ (Columba livia) intertemporal choice in binary-choice and patch-leaving contexts.3
Target number influences strategy use by rats (Rattus norvegicus) in the traveling salesperson problem.3
Egg burial in the ringneck dove (Streptopelia risoria): A potential laboratory model system for egg-rejection research?3
Effect of resource-associated signals on producer responses: Insights from golden fish (Carassius auratus) and an agent-based model.3
Supplemental Material for Sensitivity to Immature Skill Deficits. Food Sharing Experiments in Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri boliviensis) and Common Marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)3
A study of executive function in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience can affect delay of gratification.2
Supplemental Material for Inhibitory Control and Cue Relevance Modulate Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) Performance in a Spatial Foraging Task2
Supplemental Material for Morgan’s Canon and the Associative–Cognitive Distinction Today: A Survey of Practitioners2
Supplemental Material for Recognition of Visual Kinship Signals in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by Humans (Homo sapiens)2
Primate socio-ecology shapes the evolution of distinctive facial repertoires.2
Gaze in cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).2
Supplemental Material for Physiological Constraints and Cognitive Chunking: Sequence Organization in the Songs of Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata)2
Vocal repertoire and auditory sensitivity of white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula).2
Supplemental Material for Primate Socio-Ecology Shapes the Evolution of Distinctive Facial Repertoires2
Adaptable navigation in bull ants (Myrmecia midas).2
Differences in paradoxical choice between pigeons (Columba livia) and rats (Rattus norvegicus): The problem of cue trackability.2
Implementation of automated cognitive testing systems for socially housed rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and squirrel (Saimiri spp.) monkeys: Age differences in learning.2
Supplemental Material for Detouring While Foraging Up a Tree: What Bull Ants (Myrmecia midas) Learn and Their Reactions to Novel Sensory Cues2
100 years of comparative psychology advancing practice, policy, and the public—And what the future requires.1
Do that again! Memory for self-performed actions in dogs (Canis familiaris).1
Some phenomena of the cap-pushing response in honey bees (Apis mellifera spp.).1
Acknowledgment of Ad Hoc Reviewers1
Decoding the dilemma: Exploring the rules and cues of egg rejection in the American robin (Turdus migratorius) through conjoint experimentation.1
Supplemental Material for Dogs (Canis familiaris) Copy-All Refine-Later Where Children (Homo sapiens) Overimitate1
Supplemental Material for Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Exhibit Gaze Bias for Snakes Upon Hearing Alarm Calls1
Are chimpanzees futurists? Effects of motion lines and motion blur on the judgments of global motion direction in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).1
Supplemental Material for Exploring Personality Structure and Similarity in Coppery Titi Monkeys (Plecturocebus cupreus)1
Incorporating animal agency into research design could improve behavioral and neuroscience research.1
Supplemental Material for Testing the Waters: Attempts by Wild Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) to Provision People (Homo sapiens)1
The “avoid the empty cup” hypothesis does not explain great apes’ (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) responses in two three-cup one-item inference by exclusion tasks.1
Initial evidence for eliciting contrafreeloading in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) via the opportunity for playful foraging.1
Fins, feathers, fingers, and finding an explanation for the puzzle of ephemeral rewards.1
Supplemental Material for Primate Origins of Corepresentation and Cooperative Flexibility: A Comparative Study With Common Marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), Brown Capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean1
End the search quickly: Pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) share the same bias.1
Supplemental Material for Guatemalan Beaded Lizards (Helodermatidae: Heloderma charlesbogerti) Navigate and Follow a Scent Trail in Maze Tasks1
Why do distractions sometimes aid self-control? Pigeons (Columba livia) highlight possible mechanisms underlying the distraction effect.0
Testing three primate species’ attentional biases toward preferred and unpreferred foods: Seeing red or high valued food?0
Visual and acoustic exploratory behaviors toward novel stimuli in Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus) under human care.0
Beyond the information (not) given: Associative mechanisms versus representations of uncertainty in extinction in laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
Evidence of motor intentions in plants: A kinematical study.0
Very short training protocol for understanding referential cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).0
Supplemental Material for A Modified Version of the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task Tests Cognitive Flexibility in Children (Homo sapiens) and Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)0
A reflection on noncognitive factors affecting spatial cognitive testing: Examples from nonmodel species.0
Supplemental Material for Mother–Infant Relationships and Infant Independence in Wild Geoffroy’s Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi)0
Supplemental Material for Contrafreeloading in Umbrella Cockatoos (Cacatua alba): Further Evaluation of the Play Hypothesis0
Supplemental Material for Exclusion by Donkey’s Ears: Donkeys (Equus asinus) Use Acoustic Information to Find Hidden Food in a Two-Way Object-Choice Task0
Melodic and harmonic chromatic interval processing by pigeons (Columba livia).0
Supplemental Material for Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) Behavior on a Social Learning Puzzle Task Is Impacted by Sex and Demonstration but Not Oxytocin0
Same-different conceptualization in dogs (Canis familiaris).0
Supplemental Material for Hearing “Number”?: Relative Quantity Judgments Through the Echolocation by Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)0
Delayed gratification: A grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) will wait for more tokens.0
Editorial.0
On parrots, delay of gratification, executive function, and how sometimes we do the best we can.0
Behavioral and hormonal changes following social instability in young rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).0
Supplemental Material for Implementation of Automated Cognitive Testing Systems for Socially Housed Rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Squirrel (Saimiri spp.) Monkeys: Age Differences in Learning0
Supplemental Material for Influence of Group Size on Shelter Choice in Blaptica dubia Cockroaches0
Form over function: Striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis) learn arbitrary visual patterns to solve the slat-pulling task.0
No evidence of attentional bias toward threatening conspecific and allospecific faces in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) using a dot-probe task.0
Supplemental Material for Putting the Best Foot Forward: Limb Lateralization in the Goffin’s Cockatoo (Cacatua goffiniana)0
Supplemental Material for Form Over Function: Striped Skunks (Mephitis mephitis) Learn Arbitrary Visual Patterns to Solve the Slat-Pulling Task0
Supplemental Material for Are You as Fooled as I am? Visual Illusions in Human (Homo) and Nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) Primate Species0
What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication.0
The effects of positive and negative experiences on subsequent behavior and cognitive performance in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella).0
Ready Player One? Virtual environments as a tool for studying spatial cognition in nonhuman primates.0
Anthropomorphism as a contributor to the success of human (Homo sapiens) tool use.0
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) chase prey around obstacles in virtual environments.0
Supplemental Material for The Roles of Social Information, Asocial Information, and Initial Bias in Nest-Building Decisions0
The effect of delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol and methamphetamine on sustained attention in the jumping spider (Trite planiceps).0
Serial pattern learning: Pigeons (Columba livia) prefer an improving schedule over an initially easier fixed ratio schedule.0
The impact of training method on odor learning and generalization in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).0
Supplemental Material for No Same–Different Concept or Entropy Stimulus Control: Multiple-Item Array Task Performance in Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) and Pigeons (Columba livia)0
Supplemental Material for Sensitivity to Line-of-Sight in Tolerant Versus Despotic Macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta)0
The dynamics of chunking in humans (Homo sapiens) and Guinea baboons (Papio papio).0
Supplemental Material for Search Asymmetries for Threatening Faces in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)0
Supplemental Material for Pitch Affects Human (Homo sapiens) Perception of Emotional Arousal From Diverse Animal Calls0
Supplemental Material for Is Economic Risk Proneness in Young Children (Homo sapiens) Driven by Exploratory Behavior? A Comparison With Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella)0
Supplemental Material for Spatial Frequency and Global–Local Visual Processing in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and Humans (Homo sapiens)0
First report of reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus) response to human-given cues.0
Supplemental Material for Personality Structure in Wild Eastern Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)0
Supplemental Material for Impulsivity as a Trait in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris): A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis0
Positive intonation increases the perceived value of smaller rewards in a quantity discrimination task with dogs (Canis familiaris).0
Supplemental Material for Dominance in Human (Homo sapiens) Personality Space and in Hominoid Phylogeny0
The best things in life are NOT for free! More on contrafreeloading in captive parrots.0
Supplemental Material for Are Chimpanzees Futurists? Effects of Motion Lines and Motion Blur on the Judgments of Global Motion Direction in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)0
Have we met? New insights into the role of head and body cues in the visual recognition of conspecifics in gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus).0
Physiological constraints and cognitive chunking: Sequence organization in the songs of zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata).0
Selenophobia (moonlight avoidance) in nocturnal rodents: A primer.0
Supplemental Material for Eyes or Nose: Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Prefer Vision Over Olfaction When Searching for Food0
Consistent second-order motor planning by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence from a dowel task.0
The art of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) diplomacy: Unraveling the secrets of successful negotiations using machine learning.0
Supplemental Material for Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and Bonobos (Pan paniscus) Chase Prey Around Obstacles in Virtual Environments0
Responses of wild skuas (Catharacta antarctica ssp. lonnbergi) to human cues in cooperative and competitive social contexts.0
Impulsivity as a trait in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): A systematic review and meta-analysis.0
Within the sound of trouble: Do humans use pitch to correctly assess emotional arousal across species?0
Recognition of visual kinship signals in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by humans (Homo sapiens).0
Supplemental Material for Delayed Gratification: A Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) Will Wait for More Tokens0
A test of inference by exclusion in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Replication of a parrot–child comparative study using additional avian subjects.0
Dogs (Canis familiaris) copy-all refine-later where children (Homo sapiens) overimitate.0
Assessing the perception of face pareidolia in children (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).0
Individual variation in Black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) foraging behavior in response to a predator model.0
Still face in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).0
Supplemental Material for Evaluation of Decision-Making Behavior Under Uncertainty in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus apella) and Humans (Homo sapiens) Using a Modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task0
I am as fooled as you are, say some primates … but only sometimes.0
Expanding the landscape of opportunity: Professional societies support early-career researchers through community programming and peer coaching.0
Forgetting curve in spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi): Effects of delay but not the amount of information.0
What enables “distraction” to reduce delay discounting for pigeons (Columba livia).0
Supplemental Material for Variation in Neophilia in Seven Primate Species0
Supplemental Material for Effect of Resource-Associated Signals on Producer Responses: Insights From Golden Fish (Carassius auratus) and an Agent-Based Model0
Supplemental Material for Beware of My Face: The Role of Facial Configuration in Predator Recognition0
Supplemental Material for Is Inferential Reasoning a Distinctly Human Cognitive Feature? Testing Reasoning in Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)0
Primate origins of corepresentation and cooperative flexibility: A comparative study with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), brown capuchins (Sapajus apella), and Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana0
Supplemental Material for Humans’ (Homo sapiens), Capuchin Monkeys’ (Sapajus [Cebus] apella), and Rhesus Macaques’ (Macaca mulatta) Size Judgments Shift When Stimuli Change in Frequency0
Rules and metarules: Adult cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and 5-year-old children (Homo sapiens) can master both.0
Children (Homo sapiens), but not rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), perceive the one-is-more illusion.0
Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task.0
Exclusion by donkey’s ears: Donkeys (Equus asinus) use acoustic information to find hidden food in a two-way object-choice task.0
Mother–infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy’s spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi).0
Visual discrimination of size and perception of the Delboeuf illusion in the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus): A developmental disjunction?0
Supplemental Material for Sound Order Discrimination in Two Species of Birds–Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus0
Supplemental Material for The Role of Head and Body Cues in Visual Individual Recognition in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus)0
Auditory stimulation and cognitive bias in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).0
Beware of my face: The role of facial configuration in predator recognition.0
Supplemental Material for A Test of Inference by Exclusion in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Replication of a Parrot–Child Comparative Study Using Additional Avian Subjects0
No same–different concept or entropy stimulus control: Multiple-item array task performance in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) and pigeons (Columba livia).0
Cyclical variations in acoustic features within the song sessions of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae).0
Supplemental Material for Do Standard Behavioral Assays Predict Foraging Behavior of Individual Black-Capped Chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) in Response to a Predator Model or Calls?0
Supplemental Material for The Effects of Goal–Landmark Distance on Overshadowing: A Replication in Humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004)0
Supplemental Material for Cross-modal Perception of Puppies and Adult Conspecifics in Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Supplemental Material for Cyclical Variations in Acoustic Features Within the Song Sessions of Humpback Whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)0
Reach-to-grasp kinematic signatures in Colombian spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris).0
Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) behavior on a social learning puzzle task is impacted by sex and demonstration but not oxytocin.0
How do communicative cues shape the way that dogs (Canis familiaris) encode objects?0
Using isochrony, but not meter, to discriminate rhythmic sequences in rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
Sensitivity to immature skill deficits. Food sharing experiments in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri boliviensis) and common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus).0
Does comparative cognition have a WEIRD problem?0
What is it like to hear quantities? Testing dolphins on classic number estimation using a sonar setup.0
Supplemental Material for Cognitive Flexibility and Aging in Coyotes (Canis latrans)0
Are you as fooled as I am? Visual illusions in human (Homo) and nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) primate species.0
The role of head and body cues in visual individual recognition in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus).0
Supplemental Material for Still Face in Pet Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax)0
Don't let the pigeon chair the search committee: Pigeons (Columba livia) match humans’ (Homo sapiens) suboptimal approach to the secretary problem.0
Acknowledgment0
Simplicity and complexity in human and nonhuman communication.0
Responses to prey chemical cues in wild-caught, adult gopher snakes (Pituophis catenifer).0
Hearing “number”? Relative quantity judgments through the echolocation by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).0
Supplemental Material for Touching Faces: The Active Role of Infant Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) in Their Social Development0
Spatial frequency and global–local visual processing in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and humans (Homo sapiens).0
Supplemental Material for Gaze in Cats (Felis catus) and Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)0
Conflict interference in an insect.0
Sensitivity to line-of-sight in tolerant versus despotic macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta).0
Supplemental Material for Vocal Repertoire and Auditory Sensitivity of White-Throated Woodrats (Neotoma albigula)0
Supplemental Material for Positive Intonation Increases the Perceived Value of Smaller Rewards in a Quantity Discrimination Task With Dogs (Canis familiaris)0
Guatemalan beaded lizards (Helodermatidae: Heloderma charlesbogerti) navigate and follow a scent trail in maze tasks.0
A modified version of the dimensional change card sort task tests cognitive flexibility in children (Homo sapiens) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).0
Supplemental Material for Testing Three Primate Species’ Attentional Biases Toward Preferred and Unpreferred Foods: Seeing Red or High Valued Food?0
Acknowledgments0
Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.0
Together again but no need to play: Dissociating effects of isolation and separation on social interaction in female rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
Supplemental Material for Vocal Signals Produced by the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) in Highly Motivating Contexts0
Sound order discrimination in two species of birds–Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus.0
Testing the role of macaque social tolerance in ability to follow human eye gaze.0
Morgan’s canon and the associative–cognitive distinction today: A survey of practitioners.0
The roles of social information, asocial information, and initial bias in nest-building decisions.0
The coding of object thickness in plants: When roots matter.0
Supplemental Material for A Study of Executive Function in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus): Experience Can Affect Delay of Gratification0
Supplemental Material for No Evidence of Attentional Bias Toward Threatening Conspecific and Allospecific Faces in Capuchin Monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) Using a Dot-Probe Task0
Supplemental Material for Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Rank Lists Using Multiple Cognitive Mechanisms Simultaneously0
Investigation of mirror-self recognition in ravens (Corvus corax).0
Supplemental Material for Conflict Interference in an Insect0
One smell, two smells, intermixed, combined, or queued smells: What training procedure promotes the best generalization of odor detection by dogs?0
Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny.0
Putting the best foot forward: Limb lateralization in the Goffin’s cockatoo (Cacatua goffiniana).0
Variation in neophilia in seven primate species.0
Supplemental Material for Changes in Temporal Cues Elicit Rhythmic Discrimination in Rats (Rattus norvegicus)0
Factors in forgetting in spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi.0
Supplemental Material for Consistent Second-Order Motor Planning by Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence From a Dowel Task0
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) exhibit gaze bias for snakes upon hearing alarm calls.0
Changes in temporal cues elicit rhythmic discrimination in rats (Rattus norvegicus).0
0.10061502456665