Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance

Papers
(The TQCC of Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance is 3. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2020-11-01 to 2024-11-01.)
ArticleCitations
Attentional suppression of highly salient color singletons.36
Proactive enhancement and suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search.25
Attentional control and metacognitive monitoring of the effects of different types of task-irrelevant sound on serial recall.17
Fixation, flexibility, and creativity: The dynamics of mind wandering.15
Target–background segregation in a spatial interference paradigm reveals shared and specific attentional mechanisms triggered by gaze and arrows.14
Gradient activation of speech categories facilitates listeners’ recovery from lexical garden paths, but not perception of speech-in-noise.14
Introspective awareness of oculomotor attentional capture.13
Guidance of attention by working memory is a matter of representational fidelity.13
Social attention triggered by eye gaze and walking direction is resistant to temporal decay.13
Refixation patterns of mind-wandering during real-world scene perception.13
Examining the effects of goal-setting, feedback, and incentives on sustained attention.12
You cannot “count” how many items people remember in visual working memory: The importance of signal detection–based measures for understanding change detection performance.12
Twofold advantages of face processing with or without visual awareness.12
Response, rather than target detection, triggers the attentional boost effect in visual search.12
Affective priming enhances gaze cueing effect.11
The supernumerary rubber hand illusion revisited: Perceived duplication of limbs and visuotactile events.11
Statistical learning affects the time courses of salience-driven and goal-driven selection.11
Gaze elicits social and nonsocial attentional orienting: An interplay of shared and unique conflict processing mechanisms.11
A critical analysis of the functional parameters of the quiet eye using immersive virtual reality.10
Does it help to expect distraction? Attentional capture is attenuated by high distractor frequency but not by trial-to-trial predictability.10
Individual differences in perception of the speech-to-song illusion are linked to musical aptitude but not musical training.10
Ignoring the unknown: Attentional suppression of unpredictable visual distraction.10
Motor representations evoked by objects under varying action intentions.10
Multitasking strategies make the difference: Separating processing-code resources boosts multitasking efficiency when individuals prefer to interleave tasks in free concurrent dual tasking.9
Response–response bindings do not decay for 6 seconds after integration: A case for bindings’ relevance in hierarchical action control.9
The role of temporal order of relevant and irrelevant dimensions within conflict tasks.9
Representing action in terms of what not to do: Evidence for inhibitory coding during multiple action control.9
Visual illusions as a tool to hijack numerical perception: Disentangling nonsymbolic number from its continuous visual properties.9
Dynamic inhibitory control prevents salience-driven capture of visual attention.9
The eye wants what the heart wants: Female face preferences are related to partner personality preferences.9
Statistical learning of across-trial regularities during serial search.8
Liking of art and the perception of color.8
Speaking with an alien voice: Flexible sense of agency during vocal production.8
Auditory perceptual learning depends on temporal regularity and certainty.8
The outlier paradox: The role of iterative ensemble coding in discounting outliers.8
Unseeing the white bear: Negative search criteria guide visual attention through top-down suppression.8
There is more to contextual cuing than meets the eye: Improving visual search without attentional guidance toward predictable target locations.7
The time course of categorical and perceptual similarity effects in visual search.7
Thought dynamics under task demands: Evaluating the influence of task difficulty on unconstrained thought.7
Confidence can be automatically integrated across two visual decisions.7
Perceptual competition between targets and distractors determines working memory access and produces intrusion errors in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) tasks.7
Interindividual differences influence multisensory processing during spatial navigation.7
The power of the self: Anchoring information processing across contexts.7
Spatial cueing effects are not what we thought: On the timing of attentional deployment.7
Predicting phonology in language comprehension: Evidence from the visual world eye-tracking task in Mandarin Chinese.7
Social relevance modulates multisensory integration.7
It makes sense, so I see it better! Contextual information about the visual environment increases its perceived sharpness.7
On the organization of task-order and task-specific information in dual-task situations.7
Snarcing with a phone: The role of order in spatial-numerical associations is revealed by context and task demands.7
Are you looking at me? Impact of eye contact on object-based attention.6
Habituation (of attentional capture) is not what you think it is.6
Executing the homebound path is a major source of error in homing by path integration.6
A lingering question addressed: Reading rate and most efficient listening rate are highly similar.6
Causal evidence for dissociable roles of the prefrontal and superior medial frontal cortices in decision strategies.6
An SEM approach to validating the psychological model of musical groove.6
Which can explain the pip-and-pop effect during a visual search: Multisensory integration or the oddball effect?6
Past on the ground floor and future in the attic: The vertical mental timeline.6
Perception of higher-order affordances for kicking in soccer.6
Attentional suppression in time and space.6
The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants’ strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony.6
Out of sight, out of mind: Foveal processing is necessary for semantic integration of words into sentence context.6
Learned spatial suppression is not always proactive.6
Assessing the robustness of feature-based selection in visual working memory.6
Attentional priority is determined by predicted feature distributions.6
Do effects of visual contrast and font difficulty on readers’ eye movements interact with effects of word frequency or predictability?6
Task sets define boundaries of learned cognitive flexibility in list-wide proportion switch manipulations.6
Selective suppression of taboo information in visual word recognition: Evidence for cognitive control on semantics.6
Towards the boundaries of self-prioritization: Associating the self with asymmetric shapes disrupts the self-prioritization effect.6
Appealing to the cognitive miser: Using demand avoidance to modulate cognitive flexibility in cued and voluntary task switching.6
Sequence learning is surprisingly fragile in visual search.6
Habituation to onsets is controlled by spatially selective distractor expectation.5
Intentional binding: Merely a procedural confound?5
Establishing the separable contributions of spatial attention and saccade preparation across tasks with varying acuity demands.5
How feature context alters attentional template switching.5
Conflict-monitoring theory in overtime: Is temporal learning a viable explanation for the congruency sequence effect?5
Examining mechanistic explanations for ideomotor effects.5
The role of objective and introspective switch costs in voluntary task choice.5
Performance errors influence voluntary task choices.5
Me first? Positioning self in the attentional hierarchy.5
Orthographic neighborhood effects during lateralized lexical decision are abolished with bilateral presentation.5
Expectations affect the contribution of tonic global inhibition, but not of phasic global inhibition to motor imagery.5
Prioritization in visual attention does not work the way you think it does.5
Investigating how the modularity of visuospatial attention shapes conscious perception using type I and type II signal detection theory.5
Exploring task switch costs in a color-shape decision task via a mouse tracking paradigm.5
What makes a scene? Fast scene categorization as a function of global scene information at different resolutions.5
How is location defined? Implications for learning and transfer of location-specific control.5
Learning to suppress likely distractor locations in visual search is driven by the local distractor frequency.5
One-handed motor activity does not interfere with naming lateralized pictures of tools.5
The locus of proactive interference in visual working memory.5
When instructions don't help: Knowing the optimal strategy facilitates rule-based but not information-integration category learning.5
Perceptual constancy with a novel sensory skill.4
Stroking trajectory shapes velocity effects on pleasantness and other touch percepts.4
Distractor’s salience does not determine feature suppression: A commentary on Wang and Theeuwes (2020).4
Visual continuity during blinks and alterations in time perception.4
Assessing mechanisms behind crossmodal associations between visual textures and temperature concepts.4
Kinaesthetic cues when predicting the outcomes of the actions of others.4
On the automaticity of reactive item-specific control as evidenced by its efficiency under load.4
Evidence of weight-based representations of gravitational motion.4
Imagined movement accuracy is strongly associated with drivers of overt movement error and weakly associated with imagery vividness.4
Tuning the ensemble: Incidental skewing of the perceptual average through memory-driven selection.4
Dynamic in-flight shifts of working memory resources across saccades.4
Preparing for simultaneous action and inaction: Temporal dynamics and target levels of inhibitory control.4
Is zjudge a better prime for JUDGE than zudge is?: A new evaluation of current orthographic coding models.4
Scene memories are biased toward high-probability views.4
Do individual differences in face recognition ability moderate the other ethnicity effect?4
The interplay of long-term memory and working memory: When does object-color prior knowledge affect color visual working memory?4
Visual and postural eye-height information is flexibly coupled in the perception of virtual environments.4
Phonetic category activation predicts the direction and magnitude of perceptual adaptation to accented speech.4
Localizing modality compatibility effects: Evidence from dual-task interference.4
Selective attention operates on the group level for interactive biological motion.4
No evidence that self-rated negative emotion boosts visual working memory precision.4
The rise and fall of face recognition awareness across the life span.4
Asymmetric learning of dynamic spatial regularities in visual search: Robust facilitation of predictable target locations, fragile suppression of distractor locations.4
Visuoproprioceptive conflict in hand position biases tactile localization on the hand surface.4
New templates interfere with existing templates depending on their respective priority in visual working memory.4
Both target detection and response contribute to the attentional boost effect.3
Tones disrupt visual fixations and responding on a visual-spatial task.3
Are there good days and bad days for hearing? Quantifying day-to-day intraindividual speech perception variability in older and younger adults.3
Search strategies improve with practice, but not with time pressure or financial incentives.3
Visual working memory impairs visual detection: A function of working memory load or sensory load?3
Distractor–distractor interactions in visual search for oriented targets explain the increased difficulty observed in nonlinearly separable conditions.3
Auditory selective adaptation moment by moment, at multiple timescales.3
Supplemental Material for Interindividual Differences Influence Multisensory Processing During Spatial Navigation3
Selective adaptation in speech: Measuring the effects of visual and lexical contexts.3
Top-down attention control does not imply voluntary attention control for all individuals.3
Tracking flanker task dynamics: Evidence for continuous attentional selectivity.3
The tradeoff between item and order information in short-term memory does not depend on encoding time.3
Categorization templates modulate selective attention.3
The role of attention in anticipated action effects.3
Social attention as a general mechanism? Demonstrating the influence of stimulus content factors on social attentional biasing.3
The transfer of global and local processing modes.3
Evidence for initially independent monitoring of responses and response effects.3
Movement drift in optic ataxia reveals deficits in hand state estimation in oculocentric coordinates.3
Contextual cuing of visual search does not guide attention automatically in the presence of top-down goals.3
Decomposing the attentional blink.3
Simple shapes guide visual attention based on their global outline or global orientation contingent on search goals.3
Task format modulates the relationship between reading ability and Stroop interference.3
The emergence of action-effect-related motor adaptation amidst outcome unpredictability.3
Attention, task demands, and multitalker processing costs in speech perception.3
Tilt adaptation aftereffects reveal fundamental perceptual characteristics of tactile orientation processing on the hand.3
The surprising robustness of visual search against concurrent auditory distraction.3
Guidance of visual search by negative attentional templates depends on task demands.3
Auditory superiority for perceiving the beat level but not measure level in music.3
Predictive extrapolation of observed body movements is tuned by knowledge of the body biomechanics.3
May the force be against you: Better visual sensitivity to speed changes opposite to gravity.3
Outlier detection and rejection in scatterplots: Do outliers influence intuitive statistical judgments?3
Assessing the generality of strategy optimization across distinct attentional tasks.3
Oculometric indicators of individual differences in preparatory control during the antisaccade task.3
The causal role of vision in the development of spatial coordinates: Evidence from visually impaired children.3
The asymmetric mixed-category advantage in visual working memory: An attentional, not perceptual (face-specific) account.3
Sequential effects in facial attractiveness judgments using cross-classified models: Investigating perceptual and response biases.3
0.091249942779541