Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance

Papers
(The median citation count of Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance is 0. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2020-09-01 to 2024-09-01.)
ArticleCitations
Salience determines attentional orienting in visual selection.48
Attentional suppression of highly salient color singletons.35
The eye–mind wandering link: Identifying gaze indices of mind wandering across tasks.29
Proactive enhancement and suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search.23
Attentional control and metacognitive monitoring of the effects of different types of task-irrelevant sound on serial recall.16
Learned distractor rejection in the face of strong target guidance.15
Fixation, flexibility, and creativity: The dynamics of mind wandering.15
Gradient activation of speech categories facilitates listeners’ recovery from lexical garden paths, but not perception of speech-in-noise.14
Introspective awareness of oculomotor attentional capture.13
Target–background segregation in a spatial interference paradigm reveals shared and specific attentional mechanisms triggered by gaze and arrows.13
Timing is everything: Onset timing moderates the crossmodal influence of background sound on taste perception.13
Twofold advantages of face processing with or without visual awareness.12
Response, rather than target detection, triggers the attentional boost effect in visual search.12
Probabilistic cuing of visual search: Neither implicit nor inflexible.12
Refixation patterns of mind-wandering during real-world scene perception.12
Social attention triggered by eye gaze and walking direction is resistant to temporal decay.12
You cannot “count” how many items people remember in visual working memory: The importance of signal detection–based measures for understanding change detection performance.12
Guidance of attention by working memory is a matter of representational fidelity.12
Examining the effects of goal-setting, feedback, and incentives on sustained attention.11
Affective priming enhances gaze cueing effect.11
The supernumerary rubber hand illusion revisited: Perceived duplication of limbs and visuotactile events.11
Individual differences in perception of the speech-to-song illusion are linked to musical aptitude but not musical training.10
Ignoring the unknown: Attentional suppression of unpredictable visual distraction.10
Gaze elicits social and nonsocial attentional orienting: An interplay of shared and unique conflict processing mechanisms.10
A critical analysis of the functional parameters of the quiet eye using immersive virtual reality.10
The eye wants what the heart wants: Female face preferences are related to partner personality preferences.9
Representing action in terms of what not to do: Evidence for inhibitory coding during multiple action control.9
Statistical learning affects the time courses of salience-driven and goal-driven selection.9
Response–response bindings do not decay for 6 seconds after integration: A case for bindings’ relevance in hierarchical action control.9
The role of temporal order of relevant and irrelevant dimensions within conflict tasks.9
Multitasking strategies make the difference: Separating processing-code resources boosts multitasking efficiency when individuals prefer to interleave tasks in free concurrent dual tasking.9
Visual illusions as a tool to hijack numerical perception: Disentangling nonsymbolic number from its continuous visual properties.9
Dynamic inhibitory control prevents salience-driven capture of visual attention.9
Motor representations evoked by objects under varying action intentions.9
Does it help to expect distraction? Attentional capture is attenuated by high distractor frequency but not by trial-to-trial predictability.8
The outlier paradox: The role of iterative ensemble coding in discounting outliers.8
Auditory perceptual learning depends on temporal regularity and certainty.8
Statistical learning of across-trial regularities during serial search.8
Liking of art and the perception of color.8
Unseeing the white bear: Negative search criteria guide visual attention through top-down suppression.8
Speaking with an alien voice: Flexible sense of agency during vocal production.8
Sex differences in tests of mental rotation: Direct manipulation of strategies with eye-tracking.8
Only time will tell the future: Anticipatory saccades reveal the temporal dynamics of time-based location and task expectancy.7
Social relevance modulates multisensory integration.7
It makes sense, so I see it better! Contextual information about the visual environment increases its perceived sharpness.7
On the organization of task-order and task-specific information in dual-task situations.7
Snarcing with a phone: The role of order in spatial-numerical associations is revealed by context and task demands.7
Combining the senses: The role of experience- and task-dependent mechanisms in the development of audiovisual simultaneity perception.7
The time course of categorical and perceptual similarity effects in visual search.7
What can be learned in a context-specific proportion congruence paradigm? Implications for reproducibility.7
Confidence can be automatically integrated across two visual decisions.7
Predicting phonology in language comprehension: Evidence from the visual world eye-tracking task in Mandarin Chinese.7
Perceptual competition between targets and distractors determines working memory access and produces intrusion errors in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) tasks.7
Interindividual differences influence multisensory processing during spatial navigation.7
The power of the self: Anchoring information processing across contexts.7
Spatial cueing effects are not what we thought: On the timing of attentional deployment.7
Learned spatial suppression is not always proactive.6
The interplay of interval models and entrainment models in duration perception.6
Are you looking at me? Impact of eye contact on object-based attention.6
Executing the homebound path is a major source of error in homing by path integration.6
Selective suppression of taboo information in visual word recognition: Evidence for cognitive control on semantics.6
Out of sight, out of mind: Foveal processing is necessary for semantic integration of words into sentence context.6
Appealing to the cognitive miser: Using demand avoidance to modulate cognitive flexibility in cued and voluntary task switching.6
An SEM approach to validating the psychological model of musical groove.6
Habituation (of attentional capture) is not what you think it is.6
Attentional priority is determined by predicted feature distributions.6
Do effects of visual contrast and font difficulty on readers’ eye movements interact with effects of word frequency or predictability?6
Attentional suppression in time and space.6
The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants’ strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony.6
Causal evidence for dissociable roles of the prefrontal and superior medial frontal cortices in decision strategies.6
Sequence learning is surprisingly fragile in visual search.6
The visual system does not compute a single mean but summarizes a distribution.6
Past on the ground floor and future in the attic: The vertical mental timeline.6
Thought dynamics under task demands: Evaluating the influence of task difficulty on unconstrained thought.6
Perception of higher-order affordances for kicking in soccer.6
Towards the boundaries of self-prioritization: Associating the self with asymmetric shapes disrupts the self-prioritization effect.6
A lingering question addressed: Reading rate and most efficient listening rate are highly similar.6
There is more to contextual cuing than meets the eye: Improving visual search without attentional guidance toward predictable target locations.6
Assessing the robustness of feature-based selection in visual working memory.6
Performance errors influence voluntary task choices.5
Quantitative and qualitative differences in the top-down guiding attributes of visual search.5
Divergent response-time patterns in vigilance decrement tasks.5
One-handed motor activity does not interfere with naming lateralized pictures of tools.5
Learning to suppress likely distractor locations in visual search is driven by the local distractor frequency.5
Which can explain the pip-and-pop effect during a visual search: Multisensory integration or the oddball effect?5
When instructions don't help: Knowing the optimal strategy facilitates rule-based but not information-integration category learning.5
Investigating how the modularity of visuospatial attention shapes conscious perception using type I and type II signal detection theory.5
Intentional binding: Merely a procedural confound?5
Exploring task switch costs in a color-shape decision task via a mouse tracking paradigm.5
Me first? Positioning self in the attentional hierarchy.5
Establishing the separable contributions of spatial attention and saccade preparation across tasks with varying acuity demands.5
How feature context alters attentional template switching.5
The locus of proactive interference in visual working memory.5
Examining mechanistic explanations for ideomotor effects.5
The role of objective and introspective switch costs in voluntary task choice.5
What makes a scene? Fast scene categorization as a function of global scene information at different resolutions.5
Habituation to onsets is controlled by spatially selective distractor expectation.5
Causality shifts the perceived temporal order of audiovisual events.5
Orthographic neighborhood effects during lateralized lexical decision are abolished with bilateral presentation.5
Prioritization in visual attention does not work the way you think it does.5
Expectations affect the contribution of tonic global inhibition, but not of phasic global inhibition to motor imagery.5
Task sets define boundaries of learned cognitive flexibility in list-wide proportion switch manipulations.5
Eye-tracking the time course of distal and global speech rate effects.5
Preparing for simultaneous action and inaction: Temporal dynamics and target levels of inhibitory control.4
Is zjudge a better prime for JUDGE than zudge is?: A new evaluation of current orthographic coding models.4
No evidence that self-rated negative emotion boosts visual working memory precision.4
Scene memories are biased toward high-probability views.4
Do individual differences in face recognition ability moderate the other ethnicity effect?4
Set size effects in spatial updating are independent of the online/offline updating strategy.4
Evidence of weight-based representations of gravitational motion.4
Perceptual constancy with a novel sensory skill.4
Dynamic in-flight shifts of working memory resources across saccades.4
Distractor’s salience does not determine feature suppression: A commentary on Wang and Theeuwes (2020).4
Visual continuity during blinks and alterations in time perception.4
How is location defined? Implications for learning and transfer of location-specific control.4
The rise and fall of face recognition awareness across the life span.4
The interplay of long-term memory and working memory: When does object-color prior knowledge affect color visual working memory?4
On the automaticity of reactive item-specific control as evidenced by its efficiency under load.4
Visual and postural eye-height information is flexibly coupled in the perception of virtual environments.4
Localizing modality compatibility effects: Evidence from dual-task interference.4
Stroking trajectory shapes velocity effects on pleasantness and other touch percepts.4
Selective attention operates on the group level for interactive biological motion.4
Multimodal sensory integration: Diminishing returns in rhythmic synchronization.4
Assessing mechanisms behind crossmodal associations between visual textures and temperature concepts.4
Kinaesthetic cues when predicting the outcomes of the actions of others.4
Visuoproprioceptive conflict in hand position biases tactile localization on the hand surface.4
Conflict-monitoring theory in overtime: Is temporal learning a viable explanation for the congruency sequence effect?4
New templates interfere with existing templates depending on their respective priority in visual working memory.4
Tuning the ensemble: Incidental skewing of the perceptual average through memory-driven selection.4
Predictive extrapolation of observed body movements is tuned by knowledge of the body biomechanics.3
Evidence for initially independent monitoring of responses and response effects.3
Social attention as a general mechanism? Demonstrating the influence of stimulus content factors on social attentional biasing.3
Decomposing the attentional blink.3
Outlier detection and rejection in scatterplots: Do outliers influence intuitive statistical judgments?3
Top-down attention control does not imply voluntary attention control for all individuals.3
Selective adaptation in speech: Measuring the effects of visual and lexical contexts.3
The causal role of vision in the development of spatial coordinates: Evidence from visually impaired children.3
Asymmetric learning of dynamic spatial regularities in visual search: Robust facilitation of predictable target locations, fragile suppression of distractor locations.3
Both target detection and response contribute to the attentional boost effect.3
Tones disrupt visual fixations and responding on a visual-spatial task.3
What triggers a gesture? Exploring affordance compatibility effects in representational gesture production.3
Auditory superiority for perceiving the beat level but not measure level in music.3
Search strategies improve with practice, but not with time pressure or financial incentives.3
May the force be against you: Better visual sensitivity to speed changes opposite to gravity.3
The transfer of global and local processing modes.3
Assessing the generality of strategy optimization across distinct attentional tasks.3
Contextual cuing of visual search does not guide attention automatically in the presence of top-down goals.3
The asymmetric mixed-category advantage in visual working memory: An attentional, not perceptual (face-specific) account.3
More efficient shielding for internal than external attention? Evidence from asymmetrical switch costs.3
Task format modulates the relationship between reading ability and Stroop interference.3
The tradeoff between item and order information in short-term memory does not depend on encoding time.3
Categorization templates modulate selective attention.3
Imagined movement accuracy is strongly associated with drivers of overt movement error and weakly associated with imagery vividness.3
The role of attention in anticipated action effects.3
Visual working memory impairs visual detection: A function of working memory load or sensory load?3
Are there good days and bad days for hearing? Quantifying day-to-day intraindividual speech perception variability in older and younger adults.3
Distractor–distractor interactions in visual search for oriented targets explain the increased difficulty observed in nonlinearly separable conditions.3
Guidance of visual search by negative attentional templates depends on task demands.3
Movement drift in optic ataxia reveals deficits in hand state estimation in oculocentric coordinates.3
Supplemental Material for Interindividual Differences Influence Multisensory Processing During Spatial Navigation3
Oculometric indicators of individual differences in preparatory control during the antisaccade task.3
Simple shapes guide visual attention based on their global outline or global orientation contingent on search goals.3
The emergence of action-effect-related motor adaptation amidst outcome unpredictability.3
Attention, task demands, and multitalker processing costs in speech perception.3
Tilt adaptation aftereffects reveal fundamental perceptual characteristics of tactile orientation processing on the hand.3
Phonetic category activation predicts the direction and magnitude of perceptual adaptation to accented speech.3
Differential visual and auditory effects in a crossmodal induced Roelofs illusion.2
Increased perceptions of autonomy through choice fail to enhance motor skill retention.2
How does visual working memory solve the binding problem?2
Don’t look at me like that: Integration of gaze direction and facial expression.2
A cross-linguistic study of spatial parameters of eye-movement control during reading.2
Alerting effects occur in simple—But not in compound—Visual search tasks.2
Anticipatory memory for regular and random patterns.2
Global shape perception contributes to crossmodal correspondences.2
Tracking flanker task dynamics: Evidence for continuous attentional selectivity.2
Evidence of resource sharing in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm.2
The effects of recent perceptual history on stream-bounce perception.2
Auditory selective adaptation moment by moment, at multiple timescales.2
Exploring individual differences in native phonetic perception and their link to nonnative phonetic perception.2
Adaptive visual working memory: Expecting a delayed estimation task enhances visual working memory precision.2
The observer’s perspective determines which cues are used when interpreting pointing gestures.2
Can change detection succeed when change localization fails?2
Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors.2
Visual ZIP files: Viewers beat capacity limits by compressing redundant features across objects.2
The surprising robustness of visual search against concurrent auditory distraction.2
“Stay focused!”: The role of inner speech in maintaining attention during a boring task.2
The number of expected targets modulates access to working memory: A new unified account of lag-1 sparing and distractor intrusions.2
Contextual constraint and lexical competition: Revisiting biased misperception during reading.2
Orthographic forms affect speech perception in a second language: Consonant and vowel length in L2 English.2
Metric error monitoring for a cleaner record of timing.2
Redefining the decisional components of motor responses: Evidence from lexical and object decision tasks.2
The interaction of central and peripheral processes in typing and handwriting: A direct comparison.2
Dual processes underlie the effect of the Ebbinghaüs illusion on control of grasping.2
Auditory distance perception by blind and sighted participants for both within- and beyond-reach sources.2
Spontaneous perspective taking of an invisible person.2
Incidental recognition reveals attentional tradeoffs shaped by categorical similarity.2
Spatial distance of target locations affects the time course of both endogenous and exogenous attentional deployment.2
The fast–same effect of an exclusive-OR task.2
Attention shifting during the reading of Chinese sentences.2
Steering is initiated based on error accumulation.2
More of me: Self-prioritization of numeric stimuli.2
The dynamics of buffered and triggered selection from rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) streams.2
An investigation of inattentional blindness using gaze and frequency tagging.2
Thematic object pairs produce stronger and faster grouping than taxonomic pairs.2
A reassessment of the pseudoneglect effect: Attention allocation systems are selectively engaged by semantic and spatial processing.2
Reward encourages reactive, goal-directed suppression of attention.2
Task constraints act at the level of synergies and at the level of end-effector kinematics in manual reaching and manual lateral interception.2
Serial and parallel processing in multitasking: Concepts and the impact of interindividual differences on task and stage levels.2
Fractionating distraction: How past- and future-relevant distractors influence integrated decisions.2
Effects of complexity and similarity of an interruption task on resilience toward interruptions in a procedural task with sequential constraints.2
Who speaks “kid?” How experience with children does (and does not) shape the intelligibility of child speech.2
The congruency sequence effect of the Simon task in a cross-modality context.2
Visual working memory content influences correspondence processes.2
Neural supersaturation explains attentional attenuation effects on contrast appearance.2
Exploring the response code in a compatibility effect between physical size and left/right responses: The hand is more important than location.2
The contribution of consonants and vowels to auditory word recognition is shaped by language-specific properties: Evidence from Hebrew.2
Perceptual load and enumeration: Distractor interference depends on subitizing capacity.2
Sequential effects in facial attractiveness judgments using cross-classified models: Investigating perceptual and response biases.2
You read my mind: Generating and minimizing intention uncertainty under different social contexts in a two-player online game.2
Dual target search: Attention tuned to relative features, both within and across feature dimensions.2
Does order matter? Harmonic priming effects for scrambled tonal chord sequences.2
The fate of labeled and nonlabeled visual features in working memory.2
The role of affect in late perceptual processes: Evidence from bi-stable illusions, object identification, and mental rotation.2
Newly learned novel cues to location are combined with familiar cues but not always with each other.2
Attentional guidance by irrelevant features depends on their successful encoding into working memory.2
Spatial specificity of feature-based interaction between working memory and visual processing.2
Social norm learning alters feature-based visual attention: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials.1
Proactive suppression is an implicit process that cannot be summoned on demand.1
Increased display complexity reveals effects of salience in action control.1
On the difficulty of overcoming one’s accuracy bias for choosing an optimal speed–accuracy tradeoff.1
Knowledge of collision modulates defensive multisensory responses to looming insects in arachnophobes.1
Change biases identify the features that drive time perception.1
When the body matches the picture: The influence of physiological arousal on subjective familiarity of novel stimuli.1
The dominance of spatial information in object identity judgments: A persistent congruency bias even amidst conflicting statistical regularities.1
Depersonalization affects self-prioritization of bodily, but not abstract self-related information.1
Complex background information slows down parallel search efficiency by reducing the strength of interitem interactions.1
Structured visuospatial representations revealed through serial reproduction.1
On the influence of evaluation context on judgments of effort.1
Modulation of response activation leads to biases in perceptuomotor decision making.1
Sense of agency with illusory visual events.1
Is the approximate number system capacity limited? Extended display duration does not increase the limits of linear number estimation.1
Effects of false statements on visual perception hinge on social suggestibility.1
Generalizability of control across cognitive and emotional conflict.1
Statistical learning of motor preparation.1
When “looking at nothing” imparts something: Retrospective gaze cues flexibly direct prioritization in visual working memory.1
Competition and reward structures nearly eliminate time-on-task performance decrements: Implications for theories of vigilance and mental effort.1
Induced forgetting of pictures across shifts in context.1
No effect of value learning on awareness and attention for faces: Evidence from continuous flash suppression and the attentional blink.1
Examining the role of depth information in contextual cuing using a virtual reality visual search task.1
The unique effects of relatively recent conflict on cognitive control.1
0.03913688659668