Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

Papers
(The TQCC of Journal of Experimental Child Psychology is 5. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2021-05-01 to 2025-05-01.)
ArticleCitations
Editorial Board48
Children’s comparison of different-length numbers: Managing different attributes in multidigit number processing39
An experimental task to measure preschool children’s frustration induced by having to wait unexpectedly: The role of sensitivity to delay and culture32
The good, the rich, and the powerful: How young children compensate victims of moral transgressions depending on moral character, wealth, and social dominance30
What motivates early lies? Deception in 2½- to 5-year-olds27
Contribution of orthography to vocabulary acquisition in a second language: Evidence of an early word-learning advantage in elementary-school children25
The influence of language on the formation of number concepts: Evidence from preschool children who are bilingual in English and Arabic24
Find extra options or reason badly: An investigation of children’s reasoning with incompatibility statements24
School-aged children with higher anxiety symptoms show greater correspondence between subjective negative emotions and autonomic arousal24
Expressions of positive and negative shyness in preschool-age children: Temperamental correlates and visual attention to emotions23
In the presence and absence of conflicting testimony, children’s selective trust in the in-group informant in moral judgment and knowledge access22
Sound–symbol learning and the relationship to spelling in first-grade children21
Cognitive processes that underlie mathematically gifted emergent bilinguals21
Children’s evaluations of culturally diverse lunchbox foods21
Longitudinal relations between self-regulatory skills and mathematics achievement in early elementary school children from Chinese American immigrant families20
The successful use of a search strategy improves with visuospatial working memory in 2- to 4.5-year-olds19
Preschoolers negatively evaluate conventional norm violations in pretend play19
Effects of test difficulty messaging on academic cheating among middle school children19
Are facet-specific task trainings efficient in improving children’s executive functions and why (they might not be)? A multi-facet latent change score approach18
Exploring audiovisual speech perception in monolingual and bilingual children in Uzbekistan18
How to turn that frown upside down: Children make use of a listener’s facial cues to detect and (attempt to) repair miscommunication18
Trajectories of behavioral avoidance in real time: Associations with temperament and physiological dysregulation in preschoolers17
A cross-cultural comparison of finger-based and symbolic number representations17
Give yourself a hand: The role of gesture and working memory in preschoolers’ numerical knowledge17
Categories convey prescriptive information across domains and development17
Hearts, flowers, and fruits: All children need to reveal their post-error slowing17
Learning faces from variability: Four- and five-year-olds differ from older children and adults16
Predicting explicit memory for meaningful cartoons from visual paired comparison in infants and toddlers16
The role of truth and bias in parents’ judgments of children’s science interests15
Infant walking experience is related to the development of selective attention15
Finger counting as a key tool for the development of children’s numerical skills15
The factorial structure of executive functions in preschool and elementary school children and relations with intelligence15
What makes a role model motivating for young girls? The effects of the role model’s growth versus fixed mindsets about ability and interest15
What’s in a question? Parents’ question use in dyadic interactions and the relation to preschool-aged children’s math abilities15
Children’s and adults’ evaluations of self-enhancement and self-deprecation depend on the usual performance of the self-presenter15
Proud to help when i should: Children’s positive emotions following sharing decisions with a needy versus not-needy other15
Executive functions and problem-solving—The contribution of inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility to science problem-solving performance in elementary school students15
Infants’ and toddlers’ language development during the pandemic: Socioeconomic status mattered15
Children’s confidence on mathematical equivalence and fraction problems15
The effect of working memory capacity and training on intertemporal decision making in children from low-socioeconomic-status families14
Sex differences in direction giving: Are boys better than girls?14
Boys’ visuospatial abilities compensate for their relatively poor in-class attentive behavior in learning mathematics14
The animals in moral tales: Does character realism influence children’s prosocial response to stories?14
Taking account of others’ goals in social information use: Developmental changes in 3- to 7-year-old children14
Multisensory and biomechanical influences on postural control in children14
Children’s sleepiness facilitates the effect of vicarious learning on the development of fear14
Tonal interference in word learning? A comparison of Cantonese and French14
Children’s knowledge of superordinate words predicts subsequent inductive reasoning13
Mathematical skills of 11-year-old children born very preterm and full-term13
First impressions of child faces: Facial trustworthiness influences adults’ interpretations of children’s behavior in ambiguous situations13
From what I want to do to what we decided to do: 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, honor their agreements with peers13
Activity increases in empathy-related brain regions when children contribute to peers’ sadness and happiness13
Listening effort and fatigue in native and non-native primary school children13
Differences in cognitive processing? The role of verbal processes and mental effort in bilingual and monolingual children’s planning performance13
Young children’s future-oriented reasoning for self and other: Effects of conflict and perspective13
Seeing is believing: Larger Colavita effect in school-aged children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder12
Observing parental behavior in challenging tasks: Its role for goal engagement and disengagement in children12
Cognitive processing features of elementary school children with mathematical anxiety: Attentional control theory-based explanation12
Contributions of causal reasoning to early scientific literacy12
Age-related changes in children’s cognitive–motor dual tasking: Evidence from a large cross-sectional sample12
Predicting mathematics achievement from subdomains of early number competence: Differences by grade and achievement level12
Editorial Board11
Memory for emotional images across sleep versus wake in school-aged children11
The wind in the willows effect: Does age affect human versus animal faux pas recognition?11
Editorial Board11
Editorial Board11
Children’s gesture use provides insight into proportional reasoning strategies11
Sleep disturbances moderate the association between effortful control and executive functioning in early childhood11
The number line estimation task is a valid tool for assessing mathematical achievement: A population-level study with 6484 Luxembourgish ninth-graders11
Reactive and proactive cognitive control as underlying processes of number processing in children11
“Yay! Yuck!” toddlers use others’ emotional responses to reason about hidden objects11
Event-related potential correlates of implicit processing of own- and other-race faces in children11
Infants’ representation of asymmetric social influence11
Editorial Board11
Children plan manual actions similarly in structured tasks and in free play10
Siblings at home: Moderating influence of sibling presence and composition on the relation between problem behaviors and Theory of Mind in early childhood10
Examining the cognitive and perceptual perspectives of music-to-language transfer: A study of Cantonese–English bilingual children10
Modeling item-level variance of polysyllabic word reading in developing readers: Exploring semantically related child, word, and child-by-word predictors10
Mind wandering and executive dysfunction predict children’s performance in the metronome response task10
Perceptual narrowing continues throughout childhood: Evidence from specialization of face processing10
Children are eager to take credit for prosocial acts, and cost affects this tendency10
“But the poor needed it more”: Children’s judgments on procedural justice to allocate resources between two candidates equal in merit, different in need10
Associations between parents’ autonomy supportive management language and children’s science, technology, engineering, and mathematics talk during and after tinkering at home10
Mediated semantic priming interference in toddlers as seen through pupil dynamics10
Effortful control moderates the relation between negative emotionality and socially appropriate behavior10
The development of self-initiated visuo-spatial working memory10
Finger counting, finger number gesturing, and basic numerical skills: A cross-sectional study in 3- to 5-year-olds10
Social context shapes neural processing of others’ actions in 9-month-old infants10
Learning science concepts through prompts to consider alternative possible worlds10
Executive functions in deaf and hearing children: The mediating role of language skills in inhibitory control10
Quantitative or qualitative development in decision making?9
Category learning is shaped by the multifaceted development of selective attention9
The effect of feedback and recollection rejection instructions on the development of memory monitoring and accuracy9
Affordances of fractions and decimals for arithmetic among middle school students in the United States and China9
Relieved or disappointed? Children’s understanding of how others feel at the cessation of events9
Parental criticism affects adolescents’ mood and ruminative state: Self-perception appears to influence their mood response9
Young children’s metacognitive awareness of confounded evidence9
Elucidating mechanisms linking mothers’ and fathers’ mind-mindedness in infancy with children’s self-regulation at early preschool age9
Cognitive and academic growth among emergent bilingual children at risk and not at risk for math difficulties9
Preschoolers’ in-group bias promotes altruistic sharing and reduces second-party punishment: The role of theory of mind9
Infants’ intention-based evaluations of distributive actions8
Compensatory prosocial behavior in high-risk adolescents observing social exclusion: The effects of emotion feedback8
Receptive and expressive vocabulary performance in 2- to 5-year-olds in care: The role of different care types and temperament8
Bilingual children’s visual attention while reading digital picture books and story retelling8
Reading ability in children relates to rhythm perception across modalities8
Number line development of Chilean children from preschool to the end of kindergarten8
Understanding the link between theory of mind and loneliness among primary school students: A cross-lagged panel model analysis8
The effect of visual parameters on nonsymbolic numerosity estimation varies depending on the format of stimulus presentation8
Martial arts enhances working memory and attention in school-aged children: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study8
Speech rate development in Japanese-speaking children and proficiency in mora-timed rhythm8
Even young children make multiple predictions in the complex visual world8
The development of spoken word recognition in informative and uninformative sentence contexts8
Successful comparisons in novel word generalization: Executive functions or semantic knowledge?8
Editorial Board8
The relation of bilingual cognitive skills to the second language writing performance of primary grade students8
Silent word-reading fluency is strongly associated with orthotactic sensitivity among elementary school children8
Developmental patterns and computational mechanisms of school-aged children’s fairness-related decision making under social comparison8
Early presence of a “power = males” association: Girls link power to their gender less often than boys but can be as motivated to gain it8
Preschool children weigh accuracy against partisanship when seeking information8
The feeling of “kiki”: Comparing developmental changes in sound–shape correspondence for audio–visual and audio–tactile stimuli8
Balancing accuracy and speed in the development of inhibitory control8
Sources of variation in preschoolers’ relational reasoning: The interaction between language use and working memory8
Effects of praise and “easy” feedback on children’s persistence and self-evaluations8
The squeaky wheel gets the grease: Recipients’ responses influence children’s costly third-party punishment of unfairness8
Reconsidering conceptual knowledge: Heterogeneity of its components8
Relations between parental metacognitive talk and children’s early metacognition and memory7
Long-term effects of the home literacy environment on reading development: Familial risk for dyslexia as a moderator7
Cognitive processes associated with working memory in children with developmental language disorder7
Multisensory integration and maternal sensitivity are related to each other and predictive of expressive vocabulary in 24-month-olds7
Children’s understanding of counterfactual and temporal relief in others7
Building lexical networks: Preschoolers extract different types of information in cross-situational learning7
Cognitive and home predictors of precocious reading and math before formal education7
Recognition of emotional body language from dyadic and monadic point-light displays in 5-year-old children and adults7
Chinese parents’ support of preschoolers’ mathematical development7
The roots of compassion in early childhood: Relationships between theory of mind and attachment representations with empathic concern and prosocial behavior7
Editorial Board7
Infants’ short-term memory for consonant–vowel syllables7
The roles of behavioral and affective cues and false belief in children’s trait attributions7
Balanced time perspective and aggression in left-behind children: The mediating role of self-esteem7
The development of children’s and adults’ use of kinematic cues for visual anticipation and verbal prediction of action7
Editorial Board7
Parenting style and the cognitive development of preschool-aged children: Evidence from rural China7
Evaluations of epistemic and practical reasons for belief in a predominantly White U.S. sample of preschoolers7
When does it pay to follow the crowd? Children optimize imitation of causally irrelevant actions performed by a majority7
Children use race to infer who is “in charge”7
Editorial Board7
Impacts of number lines and circle visual displays on caregivers’ fraction understanding7
Moral evaluations of children’s truths and lies in a prosocial context: The role of reputation7
Longitudinal associations between parental mathematics anxiety and attitudes and young children’s mathematics attainment6
Developing mental number line games to improve young children’s number knowledge and basic arithmetic skills6
Examining the relationship between psychosocial adversity and inhibitory control: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of children growing up in extreme poverty6
A happy face advantage for pareidolic faces in children and adults6
Math talk by mothers, fathers, and toddlers: Differences across materials and associations with children’s math understanding6
Can gamification improve children’s performance in mental rotation?6
Children endorse deterrence motivations for third-party punishment but derive higher enjoyment from compensating victims6
Facial impressions of niceness influence children’s interpretations of peers’ ambiguous behavior6
The role of conventionality and design in children’s function judgments about malfunctioning artifacts6
Blurred boundaries between us and them: Do young children affiliate with outgroup members with shared preferences?6
Operational momentum during children’s approximate arithmetic relates to symbolic math skills and space–magnitude association6
Finger counting to relieve working memory in children with developmental coordination disorder: Insights from behavioral and three-dimensional motion analyses6
Preference matters: Knowledge of beneficiary’s preference influences children’s evaluations of the act of leaving a choice for others6
Testing the generalizability of minimal group attitudes in minority and majority race children6
Children’s science vocabulary uniquely predicts individual differences in science knowledge6
Turkish- and English-speaking 3-year-old children are sensitive to the evidential strength of claims when revising their beliefs6
Neural correlates of unconventional verb extensions reveal preschoolers’ analogical abilities6
Economic risk proneness in middle childhood: Uncertainty-driven exploration or novelty-seeking?6
Unveiling neurodevelopmental changes in multisensory integration while controlling attention6
Children’s developing ability to recognize deceptive use of true information6
Schadenfreude or empathy? Children’s emotional responses to the physical pain and pleasure of prosocial and antisocial others6
Negative mood induction in children: An examination across mood, physiological, and cognitive variables6
Is silence golden? A pilot study exploring associations between children’s language environment and their language skills in Estonian-speaking families6
Cross-linguistic relations of morphological awareness between Korean and English to language and reading skills for Korean adolescents6
Influence of causal language on causal understanding: A comparison between Swiss German and Turkish6
Attentional blink in infants under 7 months6
Adaptive variability in children’s conceptual models of division6
A bird’s-eye view of research practices in mathematical cognition, learning, and instruction: Reimagining the status quo6
Examining the factor structure of the home learning environment6
Editorial Board6
The development of oculomotor suppression of salient distractors in children6
Do preschoolers trust a competent robot pointer?6
Initial interactions matter: Warm-up play affects 2-year-olds’ cooperative ability with an unfamiliar same-aged peer6
The role of semantic similarity in verb learning events: Vocabulary-related changes across early development6
Majority language skill, not measures of bilingualism, predicts executive attention in bilingual children6
Rapport building with adolescents to enhance reporting and disclosure6
Exploring relative strengths in people with Down syndrome: Spatial thinking and its role in mathematics6
Dragging but not tapping promotes preschoolers’ numerical estimating with touchscreens6
Children’s expectations about the stability of others’ knowledge and preference states6
Considering the associations of adverse and positive childhood experiences with health behaviors and outcomes among emerging adults5
Where does language come from? The development of a naïve biological understanding of language5
A structural equation model of emotion knowledge and verbal intelligence in peer acceptance in a sample of Portuguese preschoolers5
Task demands matter in shaping how preschoolers express instrumental helping, comforting, and sharing: A longitudinal analysis5
Tomorrow versus a year from now: Do children represent the near and distant future differently?5
A longitudinal study of theory of mind and listening comprehension: Is preschool theory of mind important?5
Five-month-old infants attribute inferences based on general knowledge to agents5
Inside a child’s mind: The relations between mind wandering and executive function across 8- to 12-year-olds5
Dissociated contributions of working memory and inhibitory control to children’s and adults’ analogical reasoning: Analogical strategies matter5
Chatbot dialogic reading boosts comprehension for Chinese kindergarteners with higher language skills5
Give some, keep some, put some: The language of sharing in children5
High variability in learning materials benefits children’s pattern practice5
Parents’ math anxiety and mathematics performance of pre-kindergarten children5
How executive functioning, sentence processing, and vocabulary are related at 3 years of age5
Do embodiment and fictionality affect young children’s learning?5
Self-projection in early childhood: No evidence for a common underpinning of episodic memory, episodic future thinking, theory of mind, and spatial navigation5
Bilingual children judge moral, social, and language violations as less transgressive than monolingual children5
Two birds in the hand: Concurrent and switching cognitive flexibility in preschoolers5
The influence of group membership on false-belief attribution in preschool children5
Stopping at nothing: Two-year-olds differentiate between interrupted and abandoned goals5
Converging evidence for domain-general developmental trends of mental attentional capacity: Validity and reliability of full and abbreviated measures5
Perception of visual and audiovisual trajectories toward and away from the body in the first postnatal year5
Divide and conquer: Relations among arithmetic operations and emerging knowledge of fraction notation for Chinese students in Grade 45
But they weren’t being careful! Role of theory of mind in moral judgments about victim and transgressor negligence5
The profit motive: Implications for children’s reasoning about merit-based resource distribution5
Intuitive mapping between nonsymbolic quantity and observed action across development5
Pretending with realistic and fantastical stories facilitates executive function in 3-year-old children5
Being the third wheel: Toddlers use bystander learning to acquire cue-specific valence knowledge5
The effect of visuo-haptic exploration on the development of the geometric cross-sectioning ability5
Children’s consideration of collaboration and merit when making sharing decisions in private5
Beyond literal depiction: Children’s flexible understanding of pictures5
0.24559879302979